This content is shared by kind permission of the National Elf Service. Authors: Asha Patel and Sal McKeown.
‘Brilliant!’, ‘That drawing is amazing!’, ‘You are so clever’, ‘That was a really excellent story!’
We’ve all done it; given children unstinting and exaggerated praise. Teachers and parents are generous with their positive comments to encourage children and to build their confidence, but research from the Netherlands (Brummelman et al., 2017) suggests that we may be doing more harm than good.
This is a thought provoking stance because most educators and parents in the west see raising self-esteem as a desirable objective and many rely on praise as their key strategy. It seems that the relationship between self-esteem and praise is more complex than we thought.
The research published in Child Development November/December 2017 volume 88 tests the view that parents in the west tend to give too much exaggerated praise and this can have two quite different but equally damaging effects on children’s self-esteem. Praising children may be cultural. Parents from non-Western cultures, such as China, rarely praise their children say researchers Ng, Pomerantz, & Lam, 2007 while in Western countries parents are often concerned if their children exhibit low levels of confidence. Perhaps in the west we have a stereotyped view of what success and happiness look like and associate these qualities with an outgoing, confident demeanour. We expect that praise will raise self-esteem so the more praise the better, but that may not be true.
Some children are not thrilled by gushing praise. If they are praised to the hilt for what they know to be quite modest achievements, they do not value the praise. In fact, they can see it as someone being kind to them because they lack ability. They may find it patronising. They believe others do not expect them to excel so their self-esteem is reduced. This is known as the self-deflation hypothesis. When children with low self-esteem are given excessive praise over long periods of time, it does not raise their morale. They get anxious and worry that they might not be able to keep up appearances, that they will be ‘found out.’ When they do not meet the standard, they feel very let down and their self-esteem drops still further. It seems that kindly intentions lead adults unknowingly into destructive behaviours.
However, children with high esteem who are confident and outgoing can also suffer. Inflated praise feeds a sense of entitlement as they become accustomed to this flattering and false view of themselves. They believe they are extraordinary and expect others to reinforce this view. Praise feeds a burgeoning narcissism. This is the self-inflation hypothesis. We can all identify examples of public figures worldwide who thrive on and demand excessive praise. It has become part of their identity and their way of dealing with staff and public.
Methods
This research
The original data for the report When Parents’ Praise Inflates, Children’s Self-Esteem Deflates 2017 was gathered in the Netherlands over an 18 month period from autumn 2011 to spring 2013. It surveyed 120 children:
- aged 7-11 from public elementary schools
- from families from lower-to-upper middle-class communities
- equal numbers of boys and girls.
- 87% of Dutch origin
- parents – 88% of those involved were mothers aged 30 to 62
The children completed four questionnaires at 6-monthly intervals where they scored questions such as ‘I think it’s important to stand out’, ‘I am a very special person’. They scored the questions 0 to 3 where 0 = not at all true and 3 = completely true.
Home observation
The survey started with a baseline assessment of children’s narcissism and self-esteem levels.
Five weeks later, researchers visited children at home and asked parents to administer a number of mathematical exercises using a stopwatch and a scoresheet. They noted parents’ inflated and non-inflated praise.
Inflated praise was defined by the project as statements which were positive, which contained an adverb or adjective (‘You did incredibly well!’) and were general rather than specific (‘That was amazing!’).
They followed up on children’s narcissism and self-esteem levels for three additional, 6-monthly waves.
Results
- Children with lower self-esteem at the beginning of the project received more inflated praise than their peers. Perhaps this was an encouragement to try harder, perhaps as a compensatory strategy to take the edge off disappointment.
- The more often parents gave children inflated praise in the early stages, the lower a child’s self-esteem became by the end of the project.
- Children who had high levels of self-esteem at the beginning of the project and who received inflated praise became increasingly narcissistic over time.
- It seems that children have a view of themselves and identify with the elements of praise that chime with this and reject the parts they feel are untrue.
- Those who have a high opinion of themselves think ‘Yes, indeed, I am incredible’ while those with low self-esteem believe ‘I’m not at all incredible.’
Why do parents lavish praise on their children?
The authors believe that it is not always about the child. Some parents offer excessive praise: “with the purpose of making their child (and, by association, themselves) stand out.”
In fact, parents who overvalue their children’s achievements, may have narcissistic traits. They are too involved in their child’s performances and want to bask in the child’s reflected glory.
Parents who give extravagant praise to a child lacking in confidence may have suspect motives too. Perhaps it is not about giving the child a boost. It may be that by offering praise they are taking the easy option: they do not engage in critical and potentially hurtful conversations and so avoid emotional outbursts.
As the authors point out: “However, in our study, neither inflated nor [non-]inflated praise was effective in raising self-esteem, perhaps because praise is ultimately a judgment. As one expert put it, “the most notable aspect of a positive judgment is not that it is positive but that it is a judgment” (Kohn, 1993, p. 102).”
Strengths and limitations
This is a longitudinal study which focuses on inflated and non-inflated praise, analyses self-esteem and narcissism and observes children at home and school. These are all strengths.
It is a small study of a small group in the Netherlands, a Western country. The results in China might be different.
The research focuses on praise in a limited number of situations, so it is not possible to say with confidence whether parents’ inflated praise of children’s low self-esteem would lead to a continuing downward spiral.
The results focus on what people say and the precise language that they use. They do not take into account other ways that parents might identify low self-esteem: for example, body language such as slumped shoulders, bowed head, lack of eye contact, facial expression or their attitude such as enthusiasm for trying new tasks.
Discussion
While inflated praise was shown as leading to children’s lower self-esteem and higher narcissism, non-inflated praise did not. This is perhaps because it does not put pressure on the child to perform at an exceptionally high level, nor does it make them believe they are extraordinarily gifted individuals.
Instead of overpraising achievements it is better to offer realistic praise and to engage in a dialogue about the work, eliciting the child’s views and helping them to set their own standards.
Links
Primary paper
Brummelman E, Nelemans SA, Thomaes S, Orobio de Castro B. (2017) When parents’ praise inflates, children’s self-esteem deflates. Child Development 2017; 88: 1799-1809. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12936
Other references
Mueller CM, Dweck CS. Intelligence praise can undermine motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1998; 75: 33-52.
Brummelman, E., Crocker, J., & Bushman, B. J. (2016). The praise paradox: When and why praise backfires in children with low self-esteem. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 111–115. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep. 12171 Brummelman
Kohn, A. (1993). Punished by rewards: The Trouble with gold stars, incentive plans, A’s, praise, and other bribes. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.
Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Orobio de Castro, B., Overbeek, G., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). “That’s not just beautiful—that’s incredibly beautiful!”: The adverse impact of inflated praise on children with low selfesteem. Psychological Science, 25, 728–735. https://doi. org/10.1177/0956797613514251
Ng, F. F. Y., Pomerantz, E. M., & Lam, S. F. (2007). European American and Chinese parents’ responses to children’s success and failure: Implications for children’s responses. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1239–1255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1239
Brummelman, E., Nelemans S. A., Thomaes, S., Obario de Castro, B. (2017). When Parents’ Praise Inflates, Children’s Self-Esteem Deflates. Child Development, 88, 1799-1809. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12936
Please sign in or register for FREE
If you are a registered user on SEND Network, please sign in
How do you approach giving praise in your classroom? Share your ideas in the comments!
'I was interested in your research Sal. Maybe I could add something to your interesting and, for someone who likes to encourage authenticity and inner confidence in children, not so surprising findings. I am not a researcher but a (now retired) educational psychotherapist who, as well as offering individual psychotherapy to children, also worked as a supervisor and a trainer of teachers and school staff.
For some years I worked as a consultant at a pupil referral unit, and I saw first hand how the children and young people there were often very conflicted about praise. Sometimes they craved it, but frequently it would trigger them into negative reactivity and even 'acting out', as they 'railed against' the erupting thoughts that they 'weren't good', that what they had done was actually rubbish, that they were stupid and bad etc. - thoughts which were not on their mind before the 'praise' triggered them. Praise is after all a judgement - albeit a positive one, and can alert self judgements if we are feeling sensitive - although for most people this is over-ridden by the encouragement and uplift it brings.
In my trainings with staff working with insecure and vulnerable children I would therefore be careful to alert participants to how more vulnerable children can easily 'turn praise upside down', and how taking an interest in the details of something they are doing, and/or noticing and gently acknowledging observable signs of growth can be less threatening, and even help them reflect on their own inner growth.
Eg: " Of course I'll help you Sam. I was just thinking that I think that's the first time you've asked me for help. Its good to ask for help when we need it. I think you're growing stronger. What do you think?"
angelagreenwood.net